4.4 Article

Rationally Irrational: When People Do Not Correct Their Reasoning Errors Even If They Could

Journal

Publisher

AMER PSYCHOLOGICAL ASSOC
DOI: 10.1037/xge0001375

Keywords

cognitive reflection test; dual-process theory; error correction; cognitive control; expected value of control model

Ask authors/readers for more resources

Why do people sometimes fail to correct their reasoning errors? The dominant dual-process theories of reasoning explain how people detect their reasoning errors, but they do not provide sufficient explanation for how people decide to correct these errors. Through research on cognitive control, we have explored the motivational aspects of the correction process. We argue that people decide whether or not to correct an error based on the overall expected value associated with the correction, taking into consideration the perceived efficacy, reward, and effort cost.
Why is it that sometimes people do not correct their reasoning errors? The dominating dual-process theories of reasoning detail how people (fail to) detect their reasoning errors but underspecify how people decide to correct these errors once they are detected. We have unpacked the motivational aspects of the correction process here, leveraging the research on cognitive control. Specifically, we argue that when people detect an error, they decide whether or not to correct it based on the overall expected value associated with the correction-combining perceived efficacy and the reward associated with the correction while considering the cost of effort. Using a modified two-response paradigm, participants solved cognitive reflection problems twice while we manipulated the factors defining the expected value associated with correction at the second stage. In five experiments (N = 5,908), we found that answer feedback and reward increased the probability of correction while cost decreased it, relative to the control groups. These cognitive control critical factors affected the decisions to correct reasoning errors (Experiments 2 and 3) and the corrective reasoning itself (Experiments 1, 4 and 5) across a range of problems, feedbacks, types of errors (reflective or intuitive), and cost and reward manipulations pre-tested and checked in five separate studies (N = 951). Thus, some people did not correct their epistemically irrational reasoning errors because they followed the instrumentally rational principle of the expected value maximization: They were rationally irrational.

Authors

I am an author on this paper
Click your name to claim this paper and add it to your profile.

Reviews

Primary Rating

4.4
Not enough ratings

Secondary Ratings

Novelty
-
Significance
-
Scientific rigor
-
Rate this paper

Recommended

No Data Available
No Data Available